研究生: |
許靜芬 Ching-fen Hsu |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
威廉氏症候群中文語言能力之探索 Understanding Linguistic Ability in Chinese Children with Williams Syndrome |
指導教授: |
曾志朗
Ovid J.L.Tzeng 連金發 Chin-fa Lien |
口試委員: | |
學位類別: |
博士 Doctor |
系所名稱: |
人文社會學院 - 語言學研究所 Institute of Linguistics |
論文出版年: | 2005 |
畢業學年度: | 93 |
語文別: | 英文 |
論文頁數: | 243 |
中文關鍵詞: | 威廉氏症候群 、形式語意雙向分離 、中文 、與事實相反假設語氣 、命題整合 、工作記憶 |
外文關鍵詞: | Williams Syndrome, form-meaning dissociation, Chinese, counterfactual conditionals, proposition integration, working memory |
相關次數: | 點閱:2 下載:0 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
摘要
此研究探討以中文為母語的威廉氏症候群兒童的語言能力,主要基於一個假設:威廉氏症候群兒童可能在語句形式與語意表達上出現雙向分離 (double dissociation)的表現,亦即,此族群的句法能力在基因缺損之後依然相當完好,而語意能力卻有理解上的困難。過去的研究顯示威廉氏症候群有很好的語言工作記憶能力,因而許多的研究者假設此能力是導致威廉氏症候群語言能力完好的主要原因 (Wang & Bellugi, 1994; Jerrold, Baddely, & Hewes, 1999; Vicari, Brizzolara, Carlesimo, Pezzini, & Volterra, 1996; Vicari, Carlesimo, Brizzolara, and Pezzini, 1996; Karmiloff-Smith, Grant, Berthoud, Davies, Howlin, & Udwin, 1997; Robinson, Mervis, & Robinson, 2003; Laing, E., Grant, J., Thomas, M. S .C. & Karmiloff-Smith, A., in press)。換句話說,威廉氏症候群是依靠記憶或背誦在學習語言。這樣的假設似乎可以解釋他們的語法能力與語意能力之間的差異。但是,這個假設需要被檢定。
本篇論文共包含了三個不同的研究計畫:(一)與事實相反的假設語氣,包含否定詞;(二)與事實相反的假設語氣,包含『要不是』;(三)語意的命題整合。前兩個研究計畫也探討不同的年齡層對邏輯推裡的瞭解與成熟度是否有不同。
實驗結果並沒有支持雙向分離的假設,亦即,威廉氏症候群的語意理解能力相當完好,不論是與生理年齡相符合的大專組或是與心智年齡相符合的國小組。此研究可以說是支持先天模組論的論證。
This study investigated the hypothesis of selective impairment on form and meaning in language processing on individuals with Williams Syndrome. It has been known that individuals with WS have spared grammatical knowledge even with mental retardation (average IQ of 55 or below) and poor cognition. Past research also showed that individuals with WS preserved normal verbal working memory and such intact verbal ability was thought to be responsible for their relatively good language performance (Wang & Bellugi, 1994; Jerrold, Baddely, & Hewes, 1999; Vicari, Brizzolara, Carlesimo, Pezzini, & Volterra, 1996; Vicari, Carlesimo, Brizzolara, and Pezzini, 1996; Karmiloff-Smith, Grant, Berthoud, Davies, Howlin, & Udwin, 1997; Robinson, Mervis, & Robinson, 2003; Laing, E., Grant, J., Thomas, M. S .C. & Karmiloff-Smith, A., in press). With a good verbal memory but deficit cognitive ability, individuals with WS are hypothesized to rely heavily on memory abilities in learning their language. This may explain the finding that grammatical knowledge of WS individuals is strong while their semantic understanding might be weak (Zukowski, 2001; Grant, Valian, & Karmiloff-Smith, 2002). In other words, individuals with WS might have selective impairment on form and meaning in their linguistic ability. In order to examine this issue, three projects were involved.
The first was studying on counterfactual conditions with negation in Chinese. Counterfactuals, which are mismatched in form expression and meaning understanding, served as the ideal probes to test this hypothesis. The second project was still on counterfactuals, but with a clearer conditional marker yaobushi in sentence initial position. In doing so, it was claimed that the unambiguous reading of counterfactuals might cause more efficient process in individuals with WS. The third project was about proposition integration in semantics. It was interested to investigate the relationship between form and meaning in memory of individuals with WS. Are they more sensitive to grammatical forms than sentence meaning?
A general issue of developmental delay or deviant is also raised whenever populations with mental retardation are studied. The supporters of developmental delay argued that individuals with intellectual disabilities would show similar pattern on neuropsychological profiles to their mental age matches. They are just slow in development (Zigler & Balla, 1982; Zigler, 1969). On the other hand, the supporters of developmental deviant claimed that individuals with intellectual disabilities would show a dissimilar pattern to their mental age matches. They are different in development (Ellis & Cavalier, 1982; Ellis, 1969). In this set of studies, this general issue was also under investigation.
In the study of counterfactuals with negation, the constituent comparison model in sentence verification was employed (Clark and Chase, 1972; Carpenter and Just, 1975). According to this model, the number of mental operations could be calculated. We hypothesized that people might form various possible representations mentally of a complicated sentence like counterfactuals. In this case, three representations are possibly formed in mind. Two of them were form-based representations and the other was meaning-based one. Different SOA were manipulated in sentence presentation: 0-SOA (i.e. simultaneous task) and 5-sec-SOA (i.e. delayed task). Three groups in different ages were recruited for each task from the sixth graders to college students. As for individuals with WS, the SOA was unlimited. We hypothesized that under time limitation, a form-based representation was easier to be induced. The results showed that all the three age groups were highly consistent in the performance of counterfactuals. They all formed a meaning-based representation at both SOA. However, the sixth graders showed a slightly different pattern from other two groups. Meanwhile, a clear age effect was observed. The sixth graders had slowest response latency among three groups and also erred most. Individuals with WS showed a meaning-based representation as their chronological age matches of college students and mental age matches of the sixth graders.
In the study of counterfactual conditionals with yaobushi, the design and procedure were parallel to the study of counterfactual conditionals with negation. Different participants in three age groups were recruited in each task. Meanwhile, two new individuals with WS participated in this study with unlimited SOA. The results showed that all three age groups were highly consistent in forming a meaning-based representation of counterfactuals at both SOA. A clear age effect and task effect were also observed. Meanwhile, the response latency in this study was faster than the one in the study of counterfactual conditional with negation in all age groups, suggesting an unambiguous reading of counterfactuals with yaobushi were processed more efficiently and successfully. As for the individuals with WS, they also formed a meaning-based representation as their chronological age controls and mental age controls.
In the study of proposition integration in semantics, the basic rationale was based on proposition entailment. A Bransford and Franks’ (1971) paradigm in memory recognition was conducted. There were different superset sentences broken down into sub-sentences with different number of propositions and presented auditorily to college students and individuals with WS. Half of the sentences were presented in the training section and the other half were presented in the recognition section. Participants were asked to make judgment of each sentence and assigned a confidence rating value to each judgment. The results showed that individuals with WS showed similar patterns as their normal controls in false positive rates. However, their means of confidence ratings were significantly higher than controls. In order to argue against the possible confound of yes-bias tendency, scrambled sentences were lumped together in recognition with the other half sentences from supersets. The results showed that individuals with WS recognized the scrambled sentences as they had never heard before and assigned very negative values as their normal controls.
In conclusion, individuals with WS are developmental delay, but not deviant in nature. In these three projects, they showed similar patterns as their chronological age matches in proposition integration and also in counterfactual conditionals with yaobushi. Further, they showed a similar pattern as their mental age matches in counterfactual conditionals with negation. These results indicated that they have spared logical reasoning ability and spontaneous proposition integration in their mental model. Thus, it is concluded that individuals with WS do not have selective impairment on form and meaning in their linguistic ability.
Arwood, E. 1991. Semantics and Pragmatics Language Disorders. Maryland: An Aspern Publication.
Au, Terry K.-F. 1983. Chinese and English Counterfactuals: The Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis Revisited. Cognition 15.155-187.
Au, Terry K.-F. 1984. Counterfactuals: In Reply to Alfred Bloom. Cognition 17.289-302.
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z., and George, M. 2000. The Neurocognitive Profile of Williams Syndrome: A Complex Pattern of Strengths and Weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12.7-29.
Bellugi, U., P. Wang, and Jernigan. 1994. Williams Syndrome: An Unusual Neuropsychological Profile, ed. by S. Broman., Grafman, 23-56. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bellugi, U., Mills, D., Jernigan, T. Hickok, G., and Galaburda, A. 1999. Linking Cognition, Brain Structure, and Brain Function in Williams Syndrome. pp.111-136. Neurodevelopmental Disorders. (ed.) Tager-Flusberg, H. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Bishop, D., Hartley, J., and Weir, F. 1994. Why and When Do Some Language-Impaired Children Seem Talkative? A Study of Initiation in Conversations of Children with Semantic-Pragmatic Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24.177-97.
Bishop, D. 1997. Uncommon Understanding: Development and Disorders of Language Comprehension in Children. UK: Psychology Press Limited.
Bloom, A. 1981. The Linguistic Shaping of Thought: A Study in the Impact of Language on Thinking in China and the West. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bloom, A. H. 1984. Caution-The Words You Use May Affect What You Say: A Response to Au. Cognition 17.275-287.
Bransford, J. and Franks, J. 1971. The Abstraction of Linguistic Ideas. Cognitive Psychology 2.331-350.
Bransford, J. and Franks, J. 1972. The Abstraction of Linguistic Ideas: A Review. Cognition 1.213-48.
Bransford, J. Barclay, J. and Franks, J. 1972. Sentence Memory: a Constructive Versus Interpretive Approach. Cognitive Psychology 3,193-209.
Caplan, D. and Waters, G. 1999. Verbal Working Memory and Sentence Comprehension. Behavior and Brain Science 22.77-126.
Carpenter, P. 1973. Extracting Information From Counterfactual Clauses. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 12.512-21.
Carpenter, P. and Just, M. 1975. Sentence Comprehension: A Psycholinguistic Processing Model of Verification. Psychological Review 82.45-73.
Catlin, J., and Jones, N. 1976. Verifying Affirmative and Negative Sentences. Psychological Review, 83(6), 497-501.
Chao, Y.-R. 1955. Notes on Chinese Grammar and Logic, 237-49. Anwar S. Dil: Stanford University Press.
Chao, Y.-R. 1959. How Chinese Logic Operates, ed. by Anwar S.Dil, 250-9. Stanford University Press.
Clark, H. 1976. Semantics and Comprehension. Mouton Publishers.
Condon, J. 1985. Semantics and Communication. New York, London: Macmillan Publishing Company, Collier Macmillan Publishers.
Curtiss, S., Katz, W. and Tallal, P. 1992. Delay versus Deviance in the Language Acquisition of Language-Impaired Children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 35.373-383.
Ernst, T. 1995. Negation in Mandarin Chinese. Natual Language and Linguistic Theory, 13, 665-707. Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Franks, J. and Bransford, J. 1972. The Acquisition of Abstract Ideas. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 11.311-315.
Franks, J. and Bransford, J. 1974. A Brief Note on Linguistic Integration. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 13.217-219.
Franks, J. and Bransford, J. 1974. Memory for Syntactic Form as a Function of Semantic Context. Journal of Experimental Psychology 103(5).1037-1039.
Franklin, M. and S. Barten. Franklin, M. Barten S. (ed.) 1988. Child Language: A Reader. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc.
Galaburda, A. and Bellugi, U. 2000. Mutli-Level Analysis of Cortical Neuroanatomy in Williams Syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12.74-88.
Gathercole, S., and Baddeley, A. 1993. Working Memory and Language. UK: Lawrence Erlbalm Associates, Publishers.
Grant, J., Valian, V., and Karmiloff-Smith, A. 2002. A Study of relative clauses in Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Language 29.403-16.
Hintikka, J. 2002. Negation in Logic and in Natural Language. Linguistics and Philosophy, 25, 585-600. Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Hulme, C., and Mackenzie, S. 1992. Working Memory and Severe Learning Difficulties. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Iatridou, S. 2000. The Grammatical Ingredients of Counterfactuality. Linguistic Inquiry, 31(2), 231-270.
Ippolito, M. 2002. On the Temporal Dimension of Counterfactuality. pp. 237-255. In Proceedings of the 32th Annual Conference on Northeast Linguistics Society (NELS).
James, F. 1986. Semantics of the English Subjunctive. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press.
Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A.D., Hewes, A.K. 1999. Genetically Dissociated Components of Working Memory: Evidence from Down’s and Williams Syndrome. Neuropsychologia 37.637-651.
Jaszczolt, K. M. 1999. Discourse, Beliefs and Intentions: Semantic Defaults and Propositional Attitude Ascription. Elsevier Science Ltd.
Jones, W., Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., Chiles, M., Reilly, J., Lincoln, A., and Adolphs, R. 2000. Hypersociability in Williams Syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12.30-46.
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., Berthoud, I., Davies, M., Howlin, P., and Udwin, O. 1997. Language and Williams Syndrome: How Intact is "Intact"? Child Development 68.246-62.
Karmiloff-Smith, A., and Thomas, M. 2003. What Can Developmental Disorders Tell Us About The Neurocomputational Constraints That Shape Development? The Case of Williams Syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 969-990.
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Brown, J., Grice, S., and Peterson, S. 2003. Dethroning the Myth: Cognitive Dissociates and Innate Modularity in Williams Syndrome. pp. 227-242. In In Developmental Neuropsychology, 23(1&2) (Ed.) Mervis, C. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
Kazanina, N., and Philips, C. 2003. Temporal Reference Frames and the Imperfective Paradox. pp. 287-300. In Proceedings of West Cost Conference of Formal Linguistics (WCCFL). (Ed.) Garding, G., and Tsujimura, M. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Korenberg, J., Chen, X.-N., Hirota, H., Lai, Z., Bellugi, U., Burian, D., Roe, B., and Matsuoka, R. 2000. Genome Structure and Cognitive Map of Williams Syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12.89-107.
Laing, E., Grant, J., Thomas, M. S .C. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (in press). Love is….an abstract word: The influence of phonological and semantic factors on verbal short-term memory in Williams syndrome. Cortex.
Landau, B. 1996. Multiple geometric representations of objects in languages and language learners, ed. by Bloom, P. Peterson M. Nadel L. and Garrett M., 317-64. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Lenhoff, H., Wang, P., Greenberg, F., Bellugi, U. 1997. Williams Syndrome and the Brain. Scientific American. 68-73.
Levy, Y. and Bechar, T. 2003. Cognitive Lexical and Morpho-Syntactic Profiles of Israeli Children with Williams Syndrome. Cortex 39.255-271.
Liu, Lisa G. 1985. Reasoning Counterfactuality in Chinese: Are There Any Obstacles? Cognition 21.239-270.
Majerus, S. 2004. Phonological Processing in Williams Syndrome. pp.125-142. Williams Syndrome Across Languages. (Eds.) Bartke, S., and Siegmuller, J. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publication.
Majerus, S. Linden, M.V., Mulder, L., Meulemans, T., and Peters, F. 2004. Verbal Short-term Memory Reflects the Sublexical Organization of the Phonological Language Network: Evidence from an Incidental Phonotactic Learning Paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language 51.297-306.
Martin, R., and Lesch, M. 1996. Associations and Dissociations between Language Impairment and List Recall: Implications for models of STM. pp149-177. Models of Short-Term Memory. (ed.) Gathercole, S. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Mervis, C.B., Morris, C.A., Bertrand, J., and Robinson, B.F. 1999. Williams Syndrome: Findings from an Integrated Program of Research. pp.65-110. Neurodevelopmental Disorders. (ed.) Tager-Flusberg, H. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Mervis, C. 2003. Williams Syndrome: 15 Years Of Psychological Research. pp. 1-12. In Developmental Neuropsychology, 23(1&2) (Ed.) Mervis, C. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
Montgomery, J. 2000. Verbal Working Memory and Sentence Comprehension in Children with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 293-308.
Mills, D., Alvares, T., George, M., Appelbaum, L., Bellugi, U., and Neville, H. 2000. Electrophysiological Studies of Face Processing in Williams Syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12.47-64.
Nadel, L. 1999. Down Syndrome in Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. pp.197-222. Neurodevelopmental Disorders. (ed.) Tager-Flusberg, H. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Nevins, A. 2002. Counterfactuality Without Past Tense. pp. 441-450. In Proceedings of the 32th Annual Conference on Northeast Linguistics Society (NELS).
Nichols, S., Jones, W., Roman, M., Wulfeck, B., Delis, D. Reilly, J., and Bellugi, U. 2004. Mechanisms of Verbal Memory Impairment in Four Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Brain and Language 88.180-189.
Parker, J. and P. Parker. 2002. The Official Parent's Sourcebook on Williams Syndrome: A Revised and Updated Directory for the Internet Age. San Diego: ICON Group International, Inc.
Perkins, D. 1985. Reasoning as Imagination. Interchange, 6(1), 14-26. The Ontario Institute for Studies in Education.
Philips, C. 2004. Linguistics and Linking Problems. pp. 1-41. In Developmental Language Disorders: From Phenotypes to Etiologies (Eds.) Rice, M. and Warren, S. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Prasad, P., and Arunkumar, S. 2004. From Short-Term Memory to Semantics-A Computational Model. Neural Computational & Application, 13, 157-167.
Reiss, A., Eliez, S., Schmitt, E., Straus, E., Lai, Z., Jones, W., and Bellugi, U. 2000. Neuroanatomy of Williams Syndrome: A High Resolution MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12.65-73.
Robinson, B.F., Mervis, C., and Robinson, B.W. 2003. The Roles of Verbal Short-Term Memory and Working Memory in the Acquisition of Grammar by Children with Williams Syndrome. pp. 13-31. In Developmental Neuropsychology, 23(1&2) (Ed.) Mervis, C. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
Semel, E. and S. Rosner. 2003. Understanding Williams Syndrome: Behavioral Patterns and Interventions . Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sigman, M. 1999. Developmental Deficits in Children with Down Syndrome. pp.179-196. Neurodevelopmental Disorders. (ed.) Tager-Flusberg, H. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Singer, M., and Rosenburg, S. 1973. The Role of Grammatical Relations in the Abstraction of Linguistic Ideas. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 12.273-284.
Slobin, D. 2003. Language and Thought Online: Cognitive Consequences of Linguistic Relativity. pp. 157-191. In Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought. Bradford Books.
Stiles-Davis, J. Kritchevsky M. Bellugi U. Stiles-Davis, J. Kritchevsky M. Bellugi U. (ed.) 1988. Spatial Cognition: Brain Bases and Development. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Association.
Takano, Y. 1989. Methodological Problems in Cross-Cultural Studies of Linguistic Relativity. Cognition 31.141-162.
Trabasso, T. Discussion of the Papers by Bransford and Johnson and Clark, Carpenter, and Just: Language and Cognition. 439-460.
Tyler, L., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Voice, J., Stevens, T., Grant, J., Udwin, O., Davies, M., and Howlin, P. 1997. Do Individuals with Williams Syndrome Have Bizarre Semantics? Evidence for Lexical Organization Using an On-line Task. Cortex 33, 515-527.
Ullman, M. 2004. Contributions of Memory Circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model. Cognition 92.231-270.
Vicari, S., Carlesimo, G., Brizzolara, D., and Pezzini, G. 1996. Short-term Memory in Children with Williams Syndrome: A Reduced Contribution of Lexical-Semantic Knowledge to Word Span. Neuropsychologia, 34(9).919-925.
Vicari, S., Brizzolara, D., Carlesimo, G., Pezzini, G. Volterra, V. 1996. Memory Abilities in Children with Williams Syndrome. Cortex 32.503-514.
Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., and Carlesimo, G.A. 2001. Procedural Learning Deficit in Children with Williams Syndrome. Neuropsychologia 39.665-677.
Vicari, S., Caselli, M.C., Gagliardi, C., Tonucci, F. And Volterra, V. 2002. Language Acquisition in Special Populations: a Comparison between Down and Williams Syndrome. Neuropsychologia 40.2461-2470.
Volterra, V., Longobardi, E., Pezzini, G., Vicari, S., and Antenore, C. 1999. Visuo-Spatial and Linguistic Abilities in a Twin with Williams Syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 43(4).294-305.
Wang, P. and Bellugi, U. 1993. Williams Syndrome, Down Syndrome, and Cognitive Neuroscience. American Journal of Diseases of Children 147.1246-51.
Wang, P. and Bellugi, U. 1994. Evidence from Two Genetic Syndromes for a Dissociation between Verbal and Visual-Spatial Short-Term Memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 16(2).317-322.
Wu, Z.-Y. Exploring Counterfactuals in English and Chinese. 1989. University of Massachusetts.
Wu, H.-F. 1994. "If Triangles Were Circles,..."---A Study of Counterfactuals in Chinese and in English. Taipei: The Crane Publishing Co.,Ltd.
Zukowski, A. Uncovering Grammatical Competence in Children with Williams Syndrome. 2001. Boston University, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences. Ref Type: Thesis/Dissertation.