研究生: |
賴敬宗 Lai,Ching-Tsung |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
論分佈分析對早期語言習得的解釋能力:個案研究 On the Explanatory Strength of Distributional Analysis for Early Language Acquisition: A Case Study |
指導教授: |
蘇宜青
Su,Yi-Ching |
口試委員: | |
學位類別: |
碩士 Master |
系所名稱: |
人文社會學院 - 語言學研究所 Institute of Linguistics |
論文出版年: | 2008 |
畢業學年度: | 97 |
語文別: | 英文 |
論文頁數: | 80 |
中文關鍵詞: | 兒童語言習得 、分佈分析 |
外文關鍵詞: | child language acquisition, distributional analysis |
相關次數: | 點閱:2 下載:0 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
本論文探討輸入語言(input)的分佈分析(distributional analysis)對兒童語言習得過程的解釋能力。對於語言能力為先天賦予或後天習得的爭論而言,此類討論有其重要性,因多數反對語言天賦的理論(non-nativist theory)認為跨領域的模式發現能力(domain-general pattern finding skill)是促使語言發展的一項動力。因此,若輸入語言能對一位特定兒童的語言發展作出良好的預測,則反對語言天賦的理論將獲得支持,但此種理論也必須有能力解釋成人語言與兒童語言之間的差異。
本論文的研究材料為一位習得國語的男童與其母親的口語語料(自男童1;11.9歲起至2;5.28歲止,每週收集一次)。本研究以三種方式評估這對母子的語言相似度:(i)以結構(construction)出現的頻率比較個別動詞在兒童語言中的語法發展以及成人語言中的分佈形態;(ii)搜尋兒童語言中經常出現在動詞周圍的字詞,並檢查這些字詞在輸入語言中是否為提示動詞出現的可靠線索(cue);(iii)搜尋兒童模仿成人的語句時經常刪略的字詞。研究結果顯示,分佈分析僅能解釋部分語言發展過程,且兒童語言與成人語言有兩項主要差異:(i)在兒童語言中,虛詞(functional word)大部分發展較晚,即使在輸入語言中虛詞的出現十分頻繁;以及(ii)輸入語言無法預測男童大致上「偏好」說出受詞而非主詞的傾向。
本研究的發現顯示,反對語言天賦的理論不應只依賴頻率計算為語言發展做解釋。除了計算頻率,此類理論亦應考慮輸入語言中分佈上與功能上的多元程度(distributional variety and functional variety),並且提出計量「多元程度」的尺度以做出可供檢驗的預測。另外,此類理論應有能力解釋早期語言發展中抽象語法知識(例如論旨結構)與各別字詞特有現象並存的情形。
This thesis explores to which extent distributional analysis of the input can account for a child’s language acquisition process. Such an examination is important for the nature-nurture debate in the linguistic field (whether there is innate knowledge specific to language) because most non-nativist theories recognize domain-general pattern finding skill as a driving force of language development. Therefore, a non-nativist theory will be supported when the input makes good predictions of a specific child’s language development, but it should also be ready to explain the mismatches between adult’s speech and child’s speech.
The spoken data of a Mandarin-speaking boy (collected weekly from age 1;11.9 to 2;5.28) and his mother are chosen as the research material. Three kinds of methods are used to evaluate the similarity of their speech: (i) comparing the child’s syntactic development of individual verbs with the adult’s speech by counting construction frequencies; (ii) searching for frequent items neighboring verbs in the child’s speech, and seeing if the items are reliable cues for verbs’ appearance in the input; (iii) searching for frequently dropped items when the child imitated the adult’s speech. The results show that distributional analysis can only account for part of the child’s language development process, and two major discrepancies between child’s speech and adult’s speech are found: (i) most functional items are lagged in the child’s part despite their high frequencies in the input; and (ii) the boy’s general “preference” of realizing objects but not subjects is not predicted by the input.
The findings of this study suggest that non-nativist theories should not solely rely on frequency counts to account for language development. Besides frequency counts, these theories should take distributional variety and functional variety in the input into consideration, and make a measure of “variety” to make testable predictions. Also, these theories should be able to account for the coexistence of abstract syntactic knowledge (e.g. argument structure) and item-specific phenomena in early stages of language development.
Abbot-Smith, Kirsten, Elena Lieven, and Michael Tomasello. 2001. What preschool children do and do not do with ungrammatical word orders. Cognitive Development 16: 679-692.
Abbot-Smith, Kirsten, Elena Lieven, and Michael Tomasello. 2004. Training 2;6-year-olds to produce the transitive construction: The role of frequency, semantic similarity and shared syntactic distribution. Developmental Science 7: 48-55.
Abbot-Smith, Kirsten, Elena Lieven, and Michael Tomasello. 2008. Graded representations in the acquisition of English and German transitive constructions. Cognitive Development 23: 48-66.
Abbot-Smith, Kirsten and Michael Tomasello. 2006. Exemplar-learning and schematization in a usage-based account of syntactic acquisition. The Linguistic Review 23: 275-290.
Akhtar, Nameera. 1999. Acquiring basic word order: Evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure. Journal of Child Language 26: 339-356.
Allen, Stanley E. M. and Heike Schröder. 2000. Preferred argument structure in early Inuktitut spontaneous speech data. In Preferred argument structure: Grammar as architecture for function, eds. John W. Du Bois, Lorraine E. Kumpf, and William J. Ashby, 81-108. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Borer, Hagit and Kenneth Wexler. 1992. The maturation of syntax. In Parameter setting, eds. Thomas Roeper and Edwin Williams, 123-172. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.
Bowerman, Melissa. 1990. Mapping thematic roles onto syntactic functions: Are children helped by innate linking rules? Linguistics 28: 1253-1289.
Braine, Martin D. S. 1994. Is nativism sufficient? Journal of Child Language 21: 9-31.
Braine, Martin D. S. 1988. Review of Language Learnability and Language Development by S. Pinker. Journal of Child Langauge 15: 189-199.
Brown, Roger. 1973. A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Bu, Kui-Rong. 2000. Tan “V diao” zhong “diao” de yiyi [Talking about the meaning of “diao” in “V diao”]. Chinese Language Learning 5: 12-14.
Cheung, Hintat. 1998. Ping jun yu ju chang du zai zhong wen de yun yong [The application of MLU in Chinese]. The Journal of Speech-Language-Hearing Association 13: 36-48.
Childers, Jane B. and Michael Tomasello. 2001. The role of pronouns in young children’s acquisition of the English transitive construction. Developmental Psychology 37: 730-748.
Choi, Soonja. 1988. The semantic development of negation: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language 15: 517-532.
Chomsky, Noam. 1980a. Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.
Chomsky, Noam. 1980b. Comments in Piatelli-Palmarini, ed. Language and learning: The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.
Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on government and binding. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Clancy, Patricia M. 2000. The lexicon in interaction: Developmental origins of preferred argument structure in Korean. In Preferred argument structure: Grammar as architecture for function, eds. John W. Du Bois, Lorraine E. Kumpf, and William J. Ashby, 81-108. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Croft, William. 2001. Radical Construction Grammar: Syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Diessel, Holger. 2004. The acquisition of complex sentences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dominey, Peter F. and Christelle Dodane. 2003. Indeterminacy in language acquisition: The role of child directed speech and joint attention. Journal of Neurolinguistics 17: 121-145.
Drozd, Kenneth F. 1995. Child English pre-sentential negation as metalinguistic exclamatory sentence negation. Journal of Child Language 22: 583-610.
Dryer, Matthew S. 1997. Are grammatical relations universal? In Essays on language function and language type, eds. Joan Bybee, John Haiman, and Sandra A. Thompson, 115-144. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Elman, Jeffrey L., Elizabeth A. Bates, Mark H. Johnson, Annette Karmiloff-Smith, Domenico Parisi, and Kim Plunkett. 1996. Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Fisher, Cynthia. 2002. The role of abstract syntactic knowledge in language acquisition: A reply to Tomasello (2000). Cognition 82: 259-278.
Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller and Erika Hoff. 2007. Input and the acquisition of language: Three questions. In Blackwell handbook of language development, eds. Erika Hoff and Marilyn Shatz, 107-127. Malden: Blackwell.
Gerken, LouAnn. 1991. The metrical basis for children’s subjectless sentences. Journal of Memory and Language 30: 431-451.
Gertner, Yael, Cynthia Fisher, and Julie Eisengart. 2006. Learning words and rules: Abstract knowledge of word order in early sentence comprehension. Psychological Science 17: 684-691.
Gold, E. Mark. 1967. Language identification in the limit. Information and Control 16: 447-474.
Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Goldberg, Adele E. 2006. Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Goldin-Meadow, Susan. 2003. The resilience of language: What gesture creation in deaf children can tell us about how all children learn language. New York: Psychology Press.
Greenfield, Patricia Marks and Joshua H. Smith. 1976. The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.
Ko, Pei-Chun. 2005. Lun jindai hanyu “V+diao” zhong “diao” de xuhua xienxiang [Discussion on the grammaticalization of “diao” in “V+diao” in Early Mandarin Chinese]. Oriental Humanities 4.1: 187-204.
Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar: Volume one, theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Legate, Julie Anne and Charles D. Yang. 2002. Empirical re-assessment of stimulus poverty arguments. The Linguistic Review 19: 151-162.
Li, Charles and Sandra Thompson. 1981. Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference
grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Lieven, Elena V. M., Heike Behrens, Jennifer Speares, and Michael Tomasello. 2003. Early syntactic creativity: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language 30: 333-370.
Lieven, Elena V. M., Julian M. Pine, and Gillian Baldwin. 1997. Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language 24: 187-219.
MacWhinney, Brian. 2000. The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Maratsos, Michael P. and Mary Anne Chalkley. 1980. The internal language of chidren’s syntax: The ontogenesis and representation of syntactic categories. In Children’s language, vol. 2, ed. Keith E. Nelson, 127-214. New York: Gardner.
Matthews, Danielle, Elena V. M. Lieven, Anna Theakston, and Michael Tomasello. 2005. The role of frequency in the acquisition of English word order. Cognitive Development 20: 121-136.
McClure, Kathleen, Julian M. Pine, and Elena V. M. Lieven. 2006. Investigating the abstractness of children’s early knowledge of argument structure. Journal of Child Language 33: 693-720.
Mintz, Toben H. 2002. Category induction from distributional cues in an artificial language. Memory & Cognition 30: 678-686.
Mintz, Toben H. 2003. Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech. Cognition 90: 91-117.
Mintz, Toben H. 2006. Finding the verbs: Distributional cues to categories available to young learners. In Action meets word: How children learn verbs, eds. Kathy Hirsh-Pasek and Roberta Michnick Golinkoff, 31-63. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Mintz, Toben H., Elissa L. Newport, and Thomas G. Bever. 2002. The distributional structure of grammatical categories in speech to young children. Cognitive Science 26: 393-424.
Mundy, Peter, Marian Sigman, and Connie Kasari. 1990. A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 20: 115-128.
Osterling, Julie and Geraldine Dawson. 1994. Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home video-tapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24: 247-257.
Pine, Julian M., Elena V. M. Lieven, and Caroline F. Rowland. 1998. Comparing different models of the development of the English verb category. Linguistics 36: 807-30.
Pinker, Steven. 1984. Langauge learnability and language development. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Pinker, Steven. 1989. Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Pullum, Geoffrey K. and Barbara C. Scholz. 2002. Empirical assessment of stimulus poverty arguments. The Linguistic Review 19: 9-50.
Russell, James. 2004. What is language development? Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Schlesinger, Izchak M. 1979. Cognitive and linguistic structures: The case of the instrumental. Journal of Linguistics 15: 307-324.
Slobin, Dan I. 2001. Form/function relations: How do children find out what they are? In Language development: The essential readings, eds. Michael Tomasello and Elizabeth Bates, 267-289. Malden: Blackwell.
Tang, Ting-Chi. 1988. Hanyu Cifa Jufa Lunji. Taipei: Student Books.
Theakston, Anna L., Elena V. M. Lieven, Julian M. Pine, and Caroline F. Rowland. 2001. The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of verb-argument structure: An alternative account. Journal of Child Language 28: 127-152.
Tomasello, Michael. 1992a. First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Tomasello, Michael. 1992b. The social bases of language acquisition. Social Development 1: 67-87.
Tomasello, Michael. 2000. Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 74: 209-253.
Tomasello, Michael. 2001. The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development. In Language development: The essential readings, eds. Michael Tomasello and Elizabeth Bates, 295-306. Malden: Blackwell.
Tomasello, Michael. 2003. Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, Michael, Nameera Akhtar, Kelly Dodson, and Laura Rekau. 1997. Differential productivity in young children’s use of nouns and verbs. Journal of Child Language 24: 373-387.
Valian, Virginia. 1991. Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition 40: 21-81.
Van Valin, Robert D. and Randy J. Lapolla. 1997. Syntax: Structure, meaning, and function. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Wode, Henning. 1977. Four early stages in the development of L1 negation. Journal of Child Language 4: 87-102.