研究生: |
蘇美如 Su, Mei-Ru |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
語言變體之事件相關電位研究: 以閩南語次方言為例 An ERP Study of Taiwanese Tone-Sandhi Allophones |
指導教授: |
呂菁菁
Lu, Ching-Ching |
口試委員: |
金榮泰
Chin, Jung-Tai 羅明 Lo, Ming |
學位類別: |
碩士 Master |
系所名稱: |
竹師教育學院 - 臺灣語言研究與教學研究所 Taiwan Languages and Language Teaching |
論文出版年: | 2019 |
畢業學年度: | 107 |
語文別: | 中文 |
論文頁數: | 102 |
中文關鍵詞: | 閩南語 、第五調 、事件相關腦電位 、語言變體 |
外文關鍵詞: | event-related potentials, tone-sandhi, Taiwanese, subdialect |
相關次數: | 點閱:2 下載:0 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
本研究以事件相關腦電位(ERP)之實驗,探討臺灣閩南語使用者口說表現的語言變體是否足以代表其心理詞彙庫中的語言變體。
本研究實驗以圖片促發受試者的預期語音,記錄受試者對於不同次方言的變調方式之事件相關腦電位反應,同時也請他們對於所看到的圖及所聽到的音檔配對是否符合做判斷。配合圖片的語音有四類:正確答案,包含第五調之漳腔變調、正確答案,包含第五調之泉腔變調、正確答案,不包含第五調,錯誤答案。不包含第五調的正確答案和錯誤答案這兩類題目,用以作為判定該受試者的閩南語能力是否良好的依據。完成圖片促發的事件相關腦電位實驗後,請受試者進行看圖片說出其名稱,記錄其反應並計算這些口說反應的變調方式。
研究結果發現,就受試者進行看圖片說出其名稱的口說反應來看,有些受試者使用較多的漳腔變調方式,有些受試者使用較多的泉腔變調方式。但是不論他們面對圖片說出其名稱的反應使用較多的漳腔變調方式或是較多的泉腔變調方式,他們在聽到這兩類配對時,均認為是正確配對的比例相當高,表示其心理詞彙庫同時存有兩種腔調的變體。而從其腦電位的 N400 反應及溯源分析結果,都無法支持其看圖片說出其名稱的口說反應所傾向的腔調在理解時也比較容易。
這顯示當受試者口說表現偏向某一種腔調,並不代表大腦不熟悉另一種腔調。而其口說表現結果與腦電波反應結果之間並沒有一定的相關性,即便受試者在說某一個詞彙時偏向某一種腔調,並不代表其大腦對於該詞彙與那一種腔調的語音配對處理較容易。因此,可得知語言使用者的方言腔調處理歷程相當複雜,並具有個別差異,無法單從其口說表現的語言變體下定論。
In this study, to assess whether the preferred oral form, produced by each subject in off-line confrontation picture naming, is more easily processed, EEG was recorded while the subjects made the on-line picture-sound matching. Different tone-sandhi forms, used in two major Taiwanese subdialects, were manipulated to induce different responses shown in their event-related potentials. In addition to the two tone-sandhi conditions, there were two conditions irrelevant to tone-sandhi as fillers. The event-related stimuli consisted of four conditions in total.
Behavioral results in the on-line experiments showed a pretty high acceptance rate for either one of the two tone-sandhi varieties given the same picture. It is rarely revealed in their behaviors of off-line confrontation picture naming. Grouped by their preferred forms produced orally in off-line confrontation picture naming, 7 subjects tended to use subdialect A, and 15 subjects tended to use subdialect B. However, the preferred tone-sandhi condition didn’t elicit smaller N400 as predicted in each group. Furthermore, the results from source localization analyses of their event-related potentials also confirmed the N400 results.
The findings in this study that the preferred sandhi form, produced orally by each subject in off-line confrontation picture naming, was not unequivocally processed more easily than the other tone-sandhi form in on-line picture-sound matching, suggests a necessity to give a very careful consideration in delineating the boundaries of subdialects and trying to understand the mechanisms of dialectal amalgamations and merges.
涂文欽 (2011)。彰化縣閩南語方言類型與方言分區。臺灣語文研究,6(2), 111-145。
林郁靜 (2002)。麥寮方言的調查與研究-語音及詞彙初探。碩士論文,新竹教育大學臺灣語言與語文教育研究所。
洪惟仁 (1985)。臺灣河佬話語聲調研究。自立晚報社。
洪惟仁 (1991)。臺灣方言之旅。前衛出版社。
洪惟仁 (2003)。音變的動機與方向: 漳泉競爭與臺灣普通腔的形成。國立清華大學博士論文。
洪惟仁 (2009)。臺北地區閩南語的方言類型與方言分區。臺灣語文研究, (3), 239-309。
洪惟仁 (2010)。臺灣地區的語言分佈。敲開語言的窗口: 華語的使用現象 (情況與調查),1-39。
黃上輔 (2008)。臺灣閩南語次方言語音感知之腦事件相關電位研究。國立新竹教育大學碩士論文,1-68。
莊文岳 (2013)。臺灣彰化縣閩南語次方言聲韻調之調查比較。國立新竹教育大學碩士論文。
張美玲 (2017)。新竹南寮蟹仔埔語音演變。國立清華大學碩士論文。
張素蓉 (2006)。臺中縣海線地區泉州腔的漸層分布。 國立新竹教育大學碩士論文。
鄭錦全 (2006)。臺灣客家與閩南族群雜居環境的語言互動空間。全球視野下的客家與地方社會:第一屆臺灣客家研究國際研討會。
陳雅玲 (2010)。高雄市海岸地帶偏泉腔閩南語的語音變異。國立新竹教育大學碩士論文。
曹逢甫 (2013)。臺灣閩南語共同腔的浮現:語言學與社會語言學的探討。 語言暨語言學, 14(2), 457-484。
Ardal, S., Donald, M. W., Meuter, R., Muldrew, S., & Luce, M. (1990). Brain responses to semantic incongruity in bilinguals. Brain and Language, 39(2), 187-205. doi:10.1016/0093-934x(90)90011-5
Bentin, S., Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M.-H., Echallier, J.-F., & Pernier, J. (1999). ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: time course and scalp distribution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(3), 235-260.
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2008). An alternative perspective on "semantic P600" effects in language comprehension. Brain Research Reviews, 59(1), 55-73. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.05.003
Brown, C., & Hagoort, P. (1993). The processing nature of the n400 - evidence from masked priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 34-44. doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.1.34
Chwilla, D. J., Brown, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (1995). The n400 as a function of the level of processing. Psychophysiology, 32(3), 274-285. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb02956.x
Coull, J. T. (1998). Neural correlates of attention and arousal: Insights from electrophysiology, functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology. Progress in Neurobiology, 55(4), 343-361. doi:10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00011-2
Coulson, S., Federmeier, K. D., Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (2005). Right hemisphere sensitivity to word- and sentence-level context: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 31(1), 129-147. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.1.129
Coulson, S., King, J. W., & Kutas, M. (1998). Expect the unexpected: Event-related brain response to morphosyntactic violations. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13(1), 21-58. doi:10.1080/016909698386582
Curran, T. (2000). Brain potentials of recollection and familiarity. Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 923-938. doi:10.3758/bf03209340
Deacon, D., Breton, F., Ritter, W., & Vaughan, H. G. (1991). The relationship between n2 and n400 - scalp distribution, stimulus probability, and task relevance. Psychophysiology, 28(2), 185-200. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb00411.x
Deacon, D., Dynowska, A., Ritter, W., & Grose-Fifer, J. (2004). Repetition and semantic priming of nonwords: Implications for theories of N400 and word recognition. Psychophysiology, 41(1), 60-74. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.00120
Deacon, D., Hewitt, S., Yang, C.-M., & Nagata, M. (2000). Event-related potential indices of semantic priming using masked and unmasked words: evidence that the N400 does not reflect a post-lexical process. Cognitive Brain Research, 9(2), 137-146.
Deacon, D., & Shelley-Tremblay, J. (2000). How automatically is meaning accessed: A review of the effects of attention on semantic processing. Frontiers in Bioscience, 5, E82-E94. doi:10.2741/deacon
Debruille, J. B. (2007). The N400 potential could index a semantic inhibition. Brain Research Reviews, 56(2), 472-477. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.10.001
Duzel, E., Yonelinas, A. P., Mangun, G. R., Heinze, H. J., & Tulving, E. (1997). Event-related brain potential correlates of two states of conscious awareness in memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94(11), 5973-5978. doi:10.1073/pnas.94.11.5973
Dufour, S., Brunellière, A., & Nguyen, N. (2013). To what extent do we hear phonemic contrasts in a non-native regional variety? Tracking the dynamics of perceptual processing with EEG. Journal of psycholinguistic research, 42(2), 161-173.
Federmeier, K. D. (2007). Thinking ahead: The role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 44(4), 491-505. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00531.x
Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78-84.
Friederici, A. D., Pfeifer, E., & Hahne, A. (1993). EVENT-RELATED BRAIN POTENTIALS DURING NATURAL SPEECH PROCESSING - EFFECTS OF SEMANTIC, MORPHOLOGICAL AND SYNTACTIC VIOLATIONS. Cognitive Brain Research, 1(3), 183-192. doi:10.1016/0926-6410(93)90026-2
Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Early N400 development and later language acquisition. Psychophysiology, 43(1), 1-12. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00381.x
Friesen, D. C., Chung-Fat-Yim, A., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Lexical selection differences between monolingual and bilingual listeners. Brain and Language, 152, 1-13. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2015.11.001
Gomes, H., Ritter, W., Tartter, V. C., Vaughan Jr, H. G., & Rosen, J. J. (1997). Lexical processing of visually and auditorily presented nouns and verbs: evidence from reaction time and N400 priming data. Cognitive Brain Research, 6(2), 121-134.
Hagoort, P., Hald, L., Bastiaansen, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2004). Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science, 304(5669), 438-441. doi:10.1126/science.1095455
Halgren, E., Dhond, R. P., Christensen, N., Van Petten, C., Marinkovic, K., Lewine, J. D., & Dale, A. M. (2002). N400-like magnetoencephalography responses modulated by semantic context, word frequency, and lexical class in sentences. Neuroimage, 17(3), 1101-1116. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1268
Holcomb, P. J. (1993). Semantic priming and stimulus degradation - implications for the role of the n400 in language processing. Psychophysiology, 30(1), 47-61. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03204.x
Kiefer, M. (2002). The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: Further evidence for an automatic spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. Cognitive Brain Research, 13(1), 27-39.
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463-470. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01560-6
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty Years and Counting: Finding Meaning in the N400 Component of the Event-Related Brain Potential (ERP). In S. T. Fiske, D. L. Schacter, & S. E. Taylor (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology, Vol 62 (Vol. 62, pp. 621-647). Palo Alto: Annual Reviews.
Lau, E. F., Phillips, C., & Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (de)constructing the N400. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 920-933. doi:10.1038/nrn2532
McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive psychology, 18(1), 1-86.
Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M. (2005). Processing semantic anomalies in two languages: an electrophysiological exploration in both languages of Spanish-English bilinguals. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 205-220. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.010
Miglietta, S., Grimaldi, M., & Calabrese, A. (2013). conditioned allophony in speech perception: An ERP study. Brain and language, 126(3), 285-290.
Palomar-García, M.-Á., Bueichekú, E., Ávila, C., Sanjuán, A., Strijkers, K., Ventura-Campos, N., & Costa, A. (2015). Do bilinguals show neural differences with monolinguals when processing their native language? Brain and Language, 142, 36-44. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2015.01.004
Pelvig, D., Pakkenberg, H., Stark, A., & Pakkenberg, B. (2008). Neocortical glial cell numbers in human brains. Neurobiology of aging, 29(11), 1754-1762.
Pylkkänen, L., & Marantz, A. (2003). Tracking the time course of word recognition with MEG. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(5), 187-189.
Race, D., & Hillis, A. (2015). Naming.
Rauschecker, J. P., & Scott, S. K. (2009). Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nature neuroscience, 12(6), 718.
Schmitt, B., & Münte, T. u. Kutas, M.(2000). Electrophysiological estimates of the time course of semantic and phonological encoding during implicit picture naming. Psychophysiology, 37(473-484).
van Berkum, J. J. A., Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1999). Semantic integration in sentences and discourse: Evidence from the N400. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(6), 657-671. doi:10.1162/089892999563724
Worthington, D. L., & Bodie, G. D. (2017). The sourcebook of listening research: Methodology and measures: John Wiley & Sons.