研究生: |
鄭詠心 Cheng, Yung-Hsin |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
由情境知覺的觀點探討高齡與青年行人穿越道路之行為差異 An Investigation on Differences of Crossing Behavior between Younger and Elder Pedestrians: From the Viewpoint of Situation Awareness |
指導教授: |
王明揚
Wang, Min-Yang |
口試委員: |
盧俊銘
Lu,Jun-Ming 黃育信 Huang, Yu-Hsing |
學位類別: |
碩士 Master |
系所名稱: |
工學院 - 工業工程與工程管理學系 Department of Industrial Engineering and Engineering Management |
論文出版年: | 2017 |
畢業學年度: | 105 |
語文別: | 中文 |
論文頁數: | 97 |
中文關鍵詞: | 高齡行人 、情境知覺 、情境知覺評分技術(SART) 、情境知覺全面評估技術(SAGAT) |
外文關鍵詞: | Elder pedestrians, Situation awareness, Situation Awareness Rating Technique (SART), Situation Awareness Global Assessment Technique (SAGAT) |
相關次數: | 點閱:3 下載:0 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
死亡於交通意外的行人中約64.7%為65歲以上的高齡者,然而台灣高齡人口的比例僅有11.5%。這兩項懸殊的比例凸顯了高齡行人的安全問題。本研究旨在使用主觀與客觀的方法比較高齡與青年行人情境知覺的差異,釐清高齡行人如此危險的原因,並促進其安全。
本研究共招募30位青年與30位高齡參與者。青年參與者年齡介於20至30歲(平均數:23.43,標準差:1.76);高齡參與者則至少65歲(平均數:73.77,標準差:7.7)。所有的參與者皆具備正常的視覺能力。情境知覺評分技術(SART)用於評估參與者主觀的情境知覺;情境知覺全面評估技術(SAGAT)則被使用於測量參與者的客觀情境知覺。結果顯示兩組參與者SART分數無明顯差異,本研究推論這項結果與高齡者自我覺察不足有關。他們可能未完全意識到自身功能的退化,或不認為這些改變會影響他們的安全,而這些變化的影響則反映在他們SAGAT作業的表現。青年在SAGAT作業的表現顯然優於高齡者,本研究認為造成此結果的原因是青年有較好的視覺搜尋與注意力分配。相關分析的結果也顯示SART與SAGAT的結果無顯著相關。
根據研究結果,本研究認為高齡行人如此危險的理由是他們錯誤地評估自身能力,以及他們視覺搜尋與注意力分配的能力較差,對路況的觀察解讀無法同青年完整。因此,本研究建議高齡者應定期健檢,以理解自己的身體狀況。也可延長交通號誌紅燈與綠燈的時間,讓高齡者有充裕的時間觀察路況。此外,行人事故不單是行人的責任,駕駛也參與其中。所以本研究建議未來可進一步比較駕駛與行人的情境知覺,更加全面的了解與推論高齡行人穿越道路時會如此危險的原因。
Nearly 64.7% of pedestrians killed in accidents are elder ones. However, elder people merely account for 11.5% of Taiwanese population. These disproportionate percentages rise the issue of elder pedestrians’ safety. And we think poor situation awareness (SA) maybe the cause of this tragedy. In order to deal with this, this study examines the differences of SA between younger and elder pedestrians.
There were 30 younger and 30 elder participants with identical gender ratio attended to the experiment. The younger ones aged between 20 to 30 years (Mean = 23.43; SD = 1.76); elder ones were over 65 (Mean = 73.77; SD = 7.7). Each subjects had normal visual ability. Situation Awareness Rating Technique (SART) was applied to measure their subjective situation awareness while Situation Awareness Global Assessment Technique (SAGAT) was used to evaluate objective one. Elder participants report similar SART scores with younger ones. It seems that insufficient self-awareness of elderly might be the reason of this result. Elder people might not be fully aware of their age-related declines, or they don’t think these deteriorations could threaten them. However, these deficits would actually impact their safety, so their performances on SAGAT task are apparently worse than younger group. Effective visual search and attention distribution might enable younger people to scrutinize road situation, and these could be another explanation of SAGAT results. Further, correlation analysis indicates that there is no significant correlation between SART and SAGAT.
Accordingly, we conclude that elder pedestrians’ inaccurate estimation of their abilities might lead to their vulnerability. Their ineffective visual search and attention distribution have them fail to observe road situations as well as younger people. Hence we suggest elderly should have health examination regularly to track health condition. We also advise extending the duration of red and green lights. This could let elder people have much time to examine road situation, and thus enhance their safety. The future study could further compare the SA between drivers’ and pedestrians’. With this, the problem of elder pedestrians’ safety could be addressed much well.
1. 內政部統計處,內政部統計年報(人口年齡分配)。線上檢索日期:2016年5月11日。網址: http;//sowf.moi.gov.tw/stat/year/list.htm
2. 內政部警政署(2014) 行人道路交通事故及違規分析。網址: https;//www.npa.gov.tw/NPAGip/wSite/ct?xItem=71449&ctNode=12598&mp=1
3. 內政部警政署,警政統計(警政統計名詞定義)。線上檢索日期:2016年7月17日。網址: https;//www.npa.gov.tw/NPAGip/wSite/lp?ctNode=12599&xq_xCat=06&nowPage=3&pagesize=15
4. Anstey, K. J., Wood, J., Kerr, G., Caldwell, H., & Lord, S. R. (2009). Different cognitive profiles for single compared with recurrent fallers without dementia. Neuropsychology, 23(4), 500-508.
5. Arnold, C. M., Busch, A. J., Schachter, C. L., Harrison, L., & Olszynski, W. (2005). The Relationship of Intrinsic Fall Risk Factors to a Recent History of Falling in Older Women With Osteoporosis. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther Journal of Orthopaedic & Sports Physical Therapy, 35(7), 452-460
6. Asher, L., Aresu, M., Falaschetti, E., & Mindell, J. (2012). Most older pedestrians are unable to cross the road in time: A cross-sectional study. Age and Ageing, 41(5), 690-694.
7. Avineri, E., Shinar, D., & Susilo, Y. O. (2012). Pedestrians’ behaviour in cross walks: The effects of fear of falling and age. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 44(1), 30-34.
8. Baldock, M. R. J., Mathias, J. L., McLean, A. J., & Berndt, A. (2006). Self-regulation of driving and its relationship to driving ability among older adults. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 38(5), 1038-1045.
9. Bedny, G., & Meister, D. (1999). Theory of Activity and Situation Awareness. International Journal of Cognitive Ergonomics, 3(1), 63-72.
10. Bernhoft, I. M., & Carstensen, G. (2008). Preferences and behaviour of pedestrians and cyclists by age and gender. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 11(2), 83-95.
11. Crundall, D. (2016). Hazard prediction discriminates between novice and experienced drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 86, 47-58.
12. Dommes, A., Cavallo, V., & Oxley, J. (2013). Functional declines as predictors of risky street-crossing decisions in older pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 59, 135-143.
13. Dommes, A., Cavallo, V., Dubuisson, J., Tournier, I., & Vienne, F. (2014). Crossing a two-way street: Comparison of young and old pedestrians. Journal of Safety Research, 50, 27-34.
14. Dommes, A., Cavallo, V., Vienne, F., & Aillerie, I. (2012). Age-related differences in street-crossing safety before and after training of older pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 44(1), 42-47.
15. Dunbar, G., Holland, C., & Maylor, E. A. (2004). Older pedestrians: A critical review of the literature. London: Dept. for Transport.
16. Endsley, M. R. (1988). Design and evaluation for situation awareness enhancement. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics society 32nd Annual Meeting, 97-101.
17. Endsley, M. R. (1995a). Measurement of Situation Awareness in Dynamic Systems. Hum Factors Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 37(1), 65-84.
18. Endsley, M. R. (1995b). Toward a Theory of Situation Awareness in Dynamic Systems. Hum Factors Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 37(1), 32-64.
19. Ewing, R., Schieber, R. A., & Zegeer, C. V. (2003). Urban Sprawl as a Risk Factor in Motor Vehicle Occupant and Pedestrian Fatalities. Am J Public Health American Journal of Public Health, 93(9), 1541-1545.
20. Galna, B., Peters, A., Murphy, A. T., & Morris, M. E. (2009). Obstacle crossing deficits in older adults: A systematic review. Gait & Posture, 30(3), 270-275.
21. Gårder, P. (1989). Pedestrian safety at traffic signals: a study carried out with the help of a traffic conflicts technique. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 21(5), 435-444.
22. Geraghty, J., Holland, C., & Rochelle, K. (2016). Examining links between cognitive markers, movement initiation and change, and pedestrian safety in older adults. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 89, 151-159.
23. Glossary of All Eye & Vision Conditions. (n.d.). Retrieved June 8, 2016, from http//www.aoa.org/patients-and-public/eye-and-vision-problems/glossary-of-eye-and-vision-conditions?sso=y
24. Green, P. (2004). Driver distraction, telematics design, and workload managers: Safety issues and solutions.
25. Griswold, J., Fishbain, B., Washington, S., & Ragland, D. R. (2011). Visual assessment of pedestrian crashes. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 43(1), 301-306.
26. Harris, M. A., & Wolbers, T. (2014). How age-related strategy switching deficits affect wayfinding in complex environments. Neurobiology of Aging, 35(5), 1095-1102.
27. Harwood, R. H. (2001). Visual problems and falls. Age and Ageing, 30(4), 13-18.
28. Hassan, H., King, M., & Watt, K. (2015). The perspectives of older drivers on the impact of feedback on their driving behaviours: a qualitative study. Transportation research part F: traffic psychology and behaviour, 28, 25-39.
29. Holland, C. A., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1992). People's awareness of their age-related sensory and cognitive deficits and the implications for road safety. Applied Cognitive Psychology Appl. Cognit. Psychol., 6(3), 217-231.
30. Holland, C., & Hill, R. (2010). Gender differences in factors predicting unsafe crossing decisions in adult pedestrians across the lifespan: A simulation study. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42(4), 1097-1106.
31. Kaber, D., Zhang, Y., Jin, S., Mosaly, P., & Garner, M. (2012). Effects of hazard exposure and roadway complexity on young and older driver situation awareness and performance. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 15(5), 600-611.
32. Katzman, W. B., Vittinghoff, E., Ensrud, K., Black, D. M., & Kado, D. M. (2011). Increasing Kyphosis Predicts Worsening Mobility in Older Community-Dwelling Women: A Prospective Cohort Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 59(1), 96-100.
33. Klencklen, G., Després, O., & Dufour, A. (2012). What do we know about aging and spatial cognition? Reviews and perspectives. Ageing Research Reviews, 11(1), 123-135.
34. Kliegel, M., Martin, M., Mcdaniel, M. A., & Phillips, L. H. (2007). Adult Age Differences in Errand Planning: The Role of Task Familiarity and Cognitive Resources. Experimental Aging Research, 33(2), 145-161.
35. Kouabenan, D. R. (2002). Occupation, driving experience, and risk and accident perception. Journal of Risk Research, 5(1), 49-68.
36. Lascala, E. A., Johnson, F. W., & Gruenewald, P. J. (2001). Neighborhood Characteristics of Alcohol-Related Pedestrian Injury Collisions: A Geostatistical Analysis. Prevention Science, 2(2), 123-34.
37. Lobjois, R., & Cavallo, V. (2007). Age-related differences in street-crossing decisions: The effects of vehicle speed and time constraints on gap selection in an estimation task. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 39(5), 934-943.
38. Matthews, M. L. (1986). Aging and the perception of driving risk and ability. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 30(12), 1159-1163
39. Molnar, L. J., & Eby, D. W. (2008). The relationship between self-regulation and driving-related abilities in older drivers: an exploratory study. Traffic injury prevention, 9(4), 314-319.
40. Owsley, C., & Mcgwin, G. (1999). Vision Impairment and Driving. Survey of Ophthalmology, 43(6), 535-550.
41. Owsley, C., Stalvey, B. T., & Phillips, J. M. (2003). The efficacy of an educational intervention in promoting self-regulation among high-risk older drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 35(3), 393-400.
42. Oxley, J. A., Ihsen, E., Fildes, B. N., Charlton, J. L., & Day, R. H. (2005). Crossing roads safely: An experimental study of age differences in gap selection by pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 37(5), 962-971.
43. Oxley, J., Fildes, B., Ihsen, E., Charlton, J., & Day, R. (1997). Differences in traffic judgements between young and old adult pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 29(6), 839-847.
44. Paquette, M. R., & Vallis, L. A. (2010). Age-related kinematic changes in late visual-cueing during obstacle circumvention. Exp Brain Res Experimental Brain Research, 203(3), 563-574.
45. Quek, J., Pua, Y., Clark, R. A., & Bryant, A. L. (2013). Effects of thoracic kyphosis and forward head posture on cervical range of motion in older adults. Manual Therapy, 18(1), 65-71.
46. Rosano, C., Studenski, S. A., Aizenstein, H. J., Boudreau, R. M., Longstreth, W. T., & Newman, A. B. (2011). Slower gait, slower information processing and smaller prefrontal area in older adults. Age and Ageing, 41(1), 58-64.
47. Rosenbloom, T., Mandel, R., Rosner, Y., & Eldror, E. (2015). Hazard perception test for pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 79, 160-169.
48. Salmon, P. M., Stanton, N. A., Walker, G. H., Jenkins, D., Ladva, D., Rafferty, L., & Young, M. (2009). Measuring Situation Awareness in complex systems: Comparison of measures study. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 39(3), 490-500.
49. Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103(3), 403-428.
50. Salthouse, T. A., & Siedlecki, K. L. (2007). Efficiency of route selection as a function of adult age. Brain and Cognition, 63(3), 279-286.
51. Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., & Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive Functioning as a Potential Mediator of Age-Related Cognitive Decline in Normal Adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(4), 566-594.
52. Sanders, C., & Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2012). Identifying the nature of impairment in planning ability with normal aging. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34(7), 724-737.
53. Scialfa, C. T., Deschênes, M. C., Ference, J., Boone, J., Horswill, M. S., & Wetton, M. (2011). A hazard perception test for novice drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 43(1), 204-208.
54. Shkuratova, N., Morris, M. E., & Huxham, F. (2004). Effects of age on balance control during walking. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 85(4), 582-588.
55. Smith, K., & Hancock, P. A. (1995). Situation Awareness Is Adaptive, Externally Directed Consciousness. Hum Factors Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 37(1), 137-148.
56. Stoker, P., Garfinkel-Castro, A., Khayesi, M., Odero, W., Mwangi, M. N., Peden, M., & Ewing, R. (2015). Pedestrian Safety and the Built Environment: A Review of the Risk Factors. Journal of Planning Literature, 30(4), 377-392.
57. Tarko, A., & Azam, M. S. (2011). Pedestrian injury analysis with consideration of the selectivity bias in linked police-hospital data. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 43(5), 1689-1695.
58. Taylor, R. M. (1990). Situational Awareness Rating Technique(SART): The development of a tool for aircrew systems design. AGARD, Situational Awareness in Aerospace Operations 17 p(SEE N 90-28972 23-53).
59. Theeuwes, J., Alferdinck, J. W., & Perel, M. (2002). Relation between glare and driving performance. Human Factors, 44(1), 95-107.
60. Tournier, I., Dommes, A., & Cavallo, V. (2016). Review of safety and mobility issues among older pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 91, 24-35.
61. Trick, L. M., Enns, J. T., Mills, J., & Vavrik, J. (2004). Paying attention behind the wheel: A framework for studying the role of attention in driving. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 5(5), 385-424.
62. Trivedi, M. M., Gandhi, T., & McCall, J. (2007). Looking-in and looking-out of a vehicle: Computer-vision-based enhanced vehicle safety. IEEE Transactions on Intelligent Transportation Systems, 8(1), 108-120.
63. Underwood, G., Ngai, A., & Underwood, J. (2013). Driving experience and situation awareness in hazard detection. Safety Science, 56, 29-35.
64. Ventsislavova, P., Gugliotta, A., Peña-Suarez, E., Garcia-Fernandez, P., Eisman, E., Crundall, D., & Castro, C. (2016). What happens when drivers face hazards on the road? Accident Analysis & Prevention, 91, 43-54.
65. Wood, Joanne M., Richard A. Tyrrell, and Trent P. Carberry. Limitations in drivers' ability to recognize pedestrians at night. Human Factors, 47.3 (2005): 644-653.
66. Woollacott, M. H. (2000). Editorial: Systems Contributing to Balance Disorders in Older Adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 55(8), M424-M428.
67. World Health Organization. (2001). Indicators for the Minimum Data Set Project on Ageing: A Critical Review in Sub-Saharan Africa. Dar es Salaam, Tanzania. June 21–22. Geneva: WHO. Epidemiology and Burden of Disease.
68. Zegeer, C. V., & Bushell, M. (2012). Pedestrian crash trends and potential countermeasures from around the world. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 44(1), 3-11.
69. Zivotofsky, A. Z., Eldror, E., Mandel, R., & Rosenbloom, T. (2012). Misjudging Their Own Steps: Why Elderly People Have Trouble Crossing the Road. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 54(4), 600-607.