研究生: |
施葦廷 Shih, Wei-Ting |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
幼兒對社會機器人之選擇性相信研究 Children's Selective Trust in Social Robots |
指導教授: |
丘嘉慧
Chiu, Chia-Hui |
口試委員: |
林佩蓉
Lin, Pei-Jung 邱富源 Chiu, Fu-Yuan |
學位類別: |
碩士 Master |
系所名稱: |
竹師教育學院 - 幼兒教育學系 Early Childhood Education |
論文出版年: | 2019 |
畢業學年度: | 107 |
語文別: | 中文 |
論文頁數: | 79 |
中文關鍵詞: | 幼兒 、生物概念 、社會機器人 、選擇性相信 |
外文關鍵詞: | Children, Biological Concept, Social Robots, Selective Trust |
相關次數: | 點閱:3 下載:0 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
人工智慧時代來臨,社會機器人漸漸融入幼兒的生活環境中。在教育現場,藉由社會機器人幫助幼兒學習,如此新興的機器對於幼兒有何意義?因此,本研究的目的在透過幼兒的認知概念與行為表現,了解社會機器人對幼兒的意義,藉以了解幼兒對社會機器人是否具有生物特徵的認知概念,進一步探討具有生物特徵的機器人是否有影響幼兒對其的選擇性相信。在研究一,主要目的是以生物概念探討社會機器人對幼兒的意義,以一百零二名三至六歲幼兒為研究對象,調查幼兒對於社會機器人生物特徵的認知,分別為外表、行為、互動性以及生命有無等生物特徵的判斷作業,了解幼兒在生物概念的觀點上如何看待社會機器人。結果顯示80%以上小班幼兒認為機器人具有生命特徵,但只有60%至70%大中班幼兒認為機器人具有生命特徵。
在研究二,主要目的從選擇性相信的表現來看社會機器人對幼兒的意義,以一百零二名三至六歲幼兒為研究對象,探討幼兒面對成人與具有生命特徵的社會機器人所提供的訊息,將如何影響幼兒的選擇性相信。結果顯示,判斷生命特徵能力較不好的幼兒偏好選擇相信成人,判斷生命特徵能力較佳的幼兒則偏好選擇相信社會機器人。但幼兒在判斷社會機器人是否具有生命特徵出現模稜兩可的情況使得幼兒游移不定的選擇相信成人或社會機器人。
As the era of artificial intelligence coming, social robots are gradually integrated into the living environment of children. At the educational scene, using social robots to help the children improve in learning, what is the significance of such a new machine for children? Therefore, the purpose of this study is to understand the meaning of social robots to children through the cognitive concepts and behavioral expressions of children. In order to understand whether children have cognitive concepts of biometric characteristics in social robots, and to further explore whether social robots with biometrics affect children selective trust.
As the first research, the main purpose is to investigate the meaning of social robots to children in biological concepts. Taking 102 children ages 3 to 6 as subjects, investigate children’s cognition of social robots biometrics, and then judgment of life features such as appearance, behavior, interaction, and life understand how children view social robots from the perspective of biological concepts. The results have shown that more than 80% of kindergarten beginners think that robots have life characteristics, but only 60% to 70% of kindergarten junior and kindergarten senior think that robots have life characteristics.
In the second research, the main purpose is to acknowledge the meaning of social robots for children from the performance of selective trust. Taking 102 children ages 3 to 6 as subjects, explore the message when children face adults and social robots with life characteristics how it influences the selectivity of children. The results have shown children who have lower ability to judge life features choose to believe in adults; nonetheless, the higher ability children choose to believe in social robots. However, when children judge whether social robots are equipped with life characteristics in unclear situation, make the children believe in both adults and social robots.
中文
沈薇薇、林志敏、賴忠勤、徐英洲、林夏婷(2018)。應用機器人為兒童說故事之圖書館服務以國立公共資訊圖書館為例。公共圖書館研究,7,1-33。取自:https://www.nlpi.edu.tw/FileDownLoad/LibraryPublication/20180516095319489495907.pdf
周海麗,、周暉(2014)。4~ 7 歲兒童基於不同可信度特質和情景的信任判斷。心理發展與教育,30(6),570-576。
林慧麗、胡中凡、曹峰銘、黃啟泰、蔣文祁、簡惠玲(譯)(2013)。U. Goswami著。認知發展:好學的大腦(Cognitive Development:The Learning Brain)。臺北市:雙葉書廊。
馬卓婭、張優、劉靖、王異芳(2017)。信息提供者身分對學前兒童選擇性信任的影響。中國臨床心理學雜誌,25(5),820-823。取自:10.16128%2Fj.cnki.1005-3611.2017.05.006
張瓊云、魏弘貞、謝孟岑、黃麗錦、郭靜晃(譯)(2008)。J. L. Cook & G. Cook著。兒童發展(Child Development: Principles and Perspectives)。臺北市:華杏出版
張蘭萍,周暉(2011)。幼兒基於信息判斷的選擇性信任-與親社會行為及決策判斷的關係。心理發展與教育,27(1),71-75。
楊雪、龍長權(2017)。兒童選擇性信任的發展與影響因素。心理學進展,7(6),812-818。取自:https://doi.org/10.12677/ap.2017.76101
董芳武(2013)。兒童對人形機器人之感知與評價研究。設計學報,18(3),23-39。取自:file:///Users/shihweiting/Downloads/1037-4833-4-PB.pdf
蓋秀靈(2012)。幼兒選擇性信任現象的研究綜述。科教導刊,2012(中),237-239。取自:10.16400/j.cnki.kjdkz.2012.03.061
劉文、車翰博(2016)。4~5歲幼兒基於親社會特質的選擇性信任研究。兒童發展,1,34-38。
簡淑真、周育如(2005)。幼兒心智理論發展與語言發展之關係-漢語心智動詞與補語句法之檢視。教育心理學報,36(4),357-373。取自:http://dx.doi.org/10.6251/BEP.20050413
羅雅芬(譯)(2003)。U. Goswami著。兒童認知(Cognition in children)。臺北市:心理。
蘇晶(2015)。3-5歲幼兒選擇性信任現象的研究綜述。黑龍江教育學院學報,34(9),82-85。
英文
Anita, A. H., Lauren, B. A., Rebecca, A. W., & Diana L. R. (2017).Adding sound to theory of mind: comparing children’s development of mental-state understanding in the auditory and visual realms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,164, 239–249. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.07.009
Bascandziev, I., & Harris, P. L. (2014). In beauty we trust: Children prefer information from more attractive informants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32, 94-99. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12022
Bernard, S., Castelain, T., Mercier, H., Kaufmann, L., van der Henst, J., & Clément, F. (2016). The boss is always right: Preschoolers endorse the testimony of a dominant over that of a subordinate. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 152, 307-317. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.007
Bernstein, D., & Crowley, K. (2008). Searching for signs of intelligent life: An investigation of young children’s beliefs about robot intelligence. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 17, 225-247. doi:10.1080/10508400801986116
Birch, S. A., Akmal, N., & Frampton, K. L. (2010). Two‐year‐olds are vigilant of others’ non‐verbal cues to credibility. Developmental Science, 13(2), 363-369. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00906.x
Brosseau‐Liard, P., Penney, D., & Poulin‐Dubois, D. (2015). Theory of mind selectively predicts preschoolers’ knowledge‐based selective word learning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 464-475. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12107
Chen, E. E., Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2013). Children trust a consensus composed of outgroup members—but do not retain that trust. Child Development, 84(1), 269-282. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01850.x
Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Choosing your informant: weighing familiarity and recent accuracy. Developmental Science, 12(3), 426-437. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00792.x
Corriveau, K. H., Fusaro, M., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Going with the flow: Preschoolers prefer non-dissenters as informants. Psychological Science, 20(3), 372–377. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02291.x
Fernández-Llamas, C., Conde, M. A., Rodríguez-Lera, F. J., Rodríguez-Sedano, F. J., & García, F. (2018). May I teach you? Students' behavior when lectured by robotic vs. human teachers. Computers in Human Behavior, 80, 460-469. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.09.028
Fusaro, M., Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2011). The good, the strong, and the accurate: Preschoolers’ evaluations of informant attributes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 561–574. DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.06.008
Gelman, S. A., & Kremer, K. E. (1991). Understanding natural cause: Children's explanations of how objects and their properties originate. Child Development, 62(2), 396-414. doi:10.2307/1131012
Gelman, S. A., & Kremer, K. E. (1991). Understanding natural cause: Children's explanations of how objects and their properties originate. Child Development, 62(2),396-414. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)00660-D
Haberman, S. J. (1982). Analysis of dispersion of multinomial responses. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 77(379), 568-580.
Harris, P. L. (2007). Trust. Development Science, 10(1), 135-138. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00575.x
Hyun, E. J., Kim, S. Y., Jang, S., & Park, S. (2008). Comparative study of effects of language instruction program using intelligence robot and multimedia on linguistic ability of young children. Proceedings of the 17th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, pp.187-192. doi:10.1109/ROMAN.2008.4600664
Jaffer, S., & Ma, L. (2015). Preschoolers show less trust in physically disabled or obese informants. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1524. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01524
Jaswal, V. K., & Neely, L. A. (2006). Adults don’t always know best: preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychology Science, 17(9), 757-758. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x
Kachel, G., Moore, R., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Two‐year‐olds use adults' but not peers' points. Developmental Science, 21(5), 1-10. doi:10.1111/desc.12660
Kanda, T., Hirano, T., Eaton, D., & Ishiguro, H. (2004). Interactive robots as social partners and peer tutors for children: A field trial. Human–Computer Interaction, 19(1), 61-84.
Kanero, J., Geçkin, V., Oranç, C., Mamus, E., Küntay, A. C., & Göksun, T. (2018). Social Robots for Early Language Learning: Current Evidence and Future Directions. Child Development Perspectives,12(3),1-6. doi:10.1111/cdep.12277
Kinzler, K. D., Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2011). Children’s selective trust in native-accented speakers. Development Science, 14(1), 106-111. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00965.x
Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76, 1261-1277. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x
Koenig, M. A., & Sabbagh, M. A.(2013). Selective social learning: New perspective on learning from others. Development Psychology, 49(3), 399-403. doi:10.1037/a0031619
Kory-Westlund, J. M., Dickens, L., Jeong, S., Harris, P. L., DeSteno, D., & Breazeal, C. L. (2017). Children use non-verbal cues to learn new words from robots as well as people. International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction, 13, 1-9. doi:10.1016/j.ijcci.2017.04.001
Kory-Westlund, J., & Breazeal, C. (2015). The interplay of robot language level with children's language learning during storytelling. Proceedings of the Tenth Annual ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction Extended Abstracts, pp.65-66. doi:10.1145/2701973.2701989
Kory-Westlund, J., Dickens, L., Jeong, S., Harris, P., DeSteno, D., & Breazeal, C. (2015). A comparison of children learning new words from robots, tablets, & people. Proceedings of the New Friends 2015, pp.26-27. Retrieved from https://ris.utwente.nl/ws/portalfiles/portal/5533236
Landrum, A. R., & Mills, C. M. (2015). Developing expectations regarding the boundaries of expertise. Cognition, 134, 215-231. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.013
Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (2013). Informants' traits weigh heavily in young children's trust in testimony and in their epistemic inferences. Child Development, 84(4), 1253-1268. doi:10.1111/cdev.12029
Li, Q. G., Heyman, G. D., Xu, F., & Lee, K. (2014). Young children’s use of honesty as a basis for selective trust. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 117, 59-72. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.09.002
Massey, C. M., & Gelman, R. (1988). Preschooler's ability to decide whether a photographed unfamiliar object can move itself. Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 307-317. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.3.307
Miller. A. Scott. (2016). Parenting and theory of mind. Florida, USA: Oxford Scholarship.
Mioduser, D., & Levy, S. T. (2010). Making sense by building sense: Kindergarten children’s construction and understanding of adaptive robot behaviors. International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 15(2), 99-127. doi:10.1007/s10758-010-9163-9
Moriguchi, Y., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., Shimada, Y., & Itakura, S. (2011). Can young children learn words from a robot? Interaction Studies, 12(1), 107-118. doi:10.1075/is.12.1.04mor
O'Connell, L., Poulin‐Dubois, D., Demke, T., & Guay, A. (2009). Can infants use a nonhuman agent's gaze direction to establish word–object relations? Infancy, 14(4), 414-438. doi:10.1080/15250000902994073
Okita, S. Y., Ng-Thow-Hing, V., & Sarvadevabhatla, R. (2009). Learning together: ASIMO developing an interactive learning partnership with children. Proceedings of the 18th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, pp.1125-1130. doi: 10.1109/ROMAN.2009.5326135
Palmquist, C. M., Jaswal, V. K., & Rutherford, A. (2016). Success inhibits preschoolers’ ability to establish selective trust. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 152, 192-204. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.07.015
Rosengren, K. S., Gelman, S. A., Kalish, C. W., & McCormick, M. (1991). As time goes by: Children's early understanding of growth in animals. Child Development, 62(6), 1302-1320. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01607.x
Solace Shen (2015). Children’s Conceptions of the Moral Standing of a Humanoid Robot of the Here and Now (Doctoral dissertation, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington). Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/1773/35303
Tanaka, F. Cicourel, A., & Movellan, J. R. (2007). Socialization between toddlers and robots at an early childhood education center. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(46), 17954-17958. doi:10.1073/pnas.0707769104
Taylor (2013). Gender influences on children’s selective trust of adult testimony. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115. 672-690. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.04.003
Toh, E., Poh, L., Causo, A., Tzuo, P. W., Chen, I., & Yeo, S. H. (2016). A Review on the Use of Robots in Education and Young Children. Journal of Educational Technology & Society, 19(2), 148-163. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/301284225
Tummeltshammer, K. S., Wu, R., Sobel, D. M., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2014). Infants track the reliability of potential informants. Psychological Science, 25(3), 1730-1738. doi:10.1177/0956797614540178
VanderBorght, M., & Jaswal, V. K. (2009). Who knows best? Preschoolers sometimes prefer child informants over adult informants. Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 18(1), 61-71. doi:10.1002/icd.591